Become a MacRumors Supporter for $50/year with no ads, ability to filter front page stories, and private forums.
The reason for not purchasing the full album in the first place is because it is loaded with songs that I don't want.
For me, it's just as likely to be because it is loaded with songs I don't know. Rather than buy nothing because I can't decide between a couple of songs and the whole album, I can proceed to buy the songs that I already like, "risk-free". I won't regret my purchase even if I later decide the rest of the songs are worth it.

But I agree the time limit is an impediment.
 
Nope, it just says:

Computer Authorizations: 4 machines are authorized to play music purchased with this account.

(2 of those machines no longer exist)

There is no way to change it, that I can see, but I'm VERY impatient. :(

I see the same thing you do. However, I know I've done it before, so maybe it is just gone because it hasn't been a year yet. I'm not certain. According to this article, that is where it should be!

http://docs.info.apple.com/article.html?artnum=93014
 
I would rather just be able to take the computer off the list at APPLE (as they are keeping track of your authorized computers, sort of like some software does when you have to move the registration FLASH, DREAMWEAVER, ETC.). I had a hard drive crash on my fiancees laptop, replaced the hard drive and boom... I lost an authorization. I had my computer at work authorized, and it was swapped out while I was on Vacation. All of my music was on the new computer, but I had to eat another autorization in order to listen to my music at work. That is two less computers I'll ever be able to hook up to again.

Does anybody know a work around? Can I call and fix the problem?

I'm wondering if you might have to make a new account and re-authorize?
that would suck. call apple and say "HEY! <fill in more words here>"
 
This is a neat feature, but I don't understand why people are upset that it wasn't offered before.

I mean, say you bought a 45 or a cassette or CD single back in the day . . . how many stores would then give you a discount or let you trade it in towards the full album or cassette? Not one.
The Beatles used to leave their Singles off their albums so their fans wouldn't have to pay for the same song twice (the album always came after the singles)... those Singles eventually became The Past Masters 1 and 2.
 
You expect iTune prices to change with the current exchange rates?? Dude, are you aware that theres such things as exchange rates and they change over time? The reason the tunes are cheaper in the US is because their economy isnt doing so well and they have a weak dollar, so when it comes to exchange rates you get a lot of dollars for your pound. If the dollar was doing well against the pound then you would see that the prices would be closer together.

Plus in Europe taxes are included... in the US are not...

people just like to complain too much...

I cannot believe this post has negatives... what the h*ll?
 
Nope, it just says:

Computer Authorizations: 4 machines are authorized to play music purchased with this account.

(2 of those machines no longer exist)

There is no way to change it, that I can see, but I'm VERY impatient. :(

I am not sure how it works, but I know that after a certain period of time (i think 12 months) the authorized computers that havent gotten online during that time, get automatically deauthorize... call Apple... I know there is a walkaround...
 
I actually like the idea of remote deauthorization, but there are some variables one must consider that puts a few holes in this. In order to deauthorize, the computer must first be connected to the internet, but must also be connected through iTunes. If you are remotely deauthorizing computers the other 4 must all be connected to iTunes so that you can select which one to deauthorize and have it happen on the spot.

If you lose that computer, or it's stolen, the theif must be connected to the online store in order for you to remote deauthorize that system. If that computer never accesses the internet again, you cannot get that one available spot in your account back. If the computer is destroyed, this is also not Apple's responsibility, and you must suffer this loss.

I'm not sure I agree with the last part. If this happens, you contact Apple and they reset your account and wipe out the authorizations (as they can do now). Then you reauthorize the machines you do have. I had this done when Apple exchanged a laptop I had and I forgot to deauthorize it.

The other thing I'd do is change your password so if the thief ever DOES connect to the net again, not only is the machine not authorized but he can't make purchases on your account without knowing your new password. Changing all passwords though is just standard security procedure for such a thing.

The problem I have with remote authorization/deauthorization though is not the possibility of losing a machine. The real problem is the security holes it opens up. Remote authorizations could be exploited for other purposes. Also, the idea of being able to change the authorization key for iTunes remotely indirectly thwarts the theoretical purpose of DRM, actual usefulness notwithstanding. In this case, the real solution might as well be to seek the abolishment of DRM entirely.
 
Yeah, remote deauthorization strikes me as a rather risky way to manage your computer accounts. It would certainly suck to suddenly have all of your iTunes-derived media suddenly be cripped because you were spoofed and your password was changed on you.
 
I'm not a 'self-proclaimed' audiophile I'm a musician and a producer and readily agree that 128K AAC is a clean bright and polished sound, equivelant in my opinion to 256K MP3. I will admit it's very difficult if not nearly impossible to discern artifaction but when you listen to a 128K AAC and then the exact same file at Apple Lossless ( or even 256K AAC) you will notice a difference in presence. All of the tangible differences are incredibly subtle but you tend to notice it in reverb, depth of soundstage and the 'tonal' quality of the treble and just the general cohesiveness of the mix...

I appreciate the feedback but anecdotal evidence (regardless of who its from) is next to worthless when speaking of differences that should be objectively discernable not just by anyone but by any system designed to quantify such discrepancies.

I too am a musician and a producer. I too use sophisticated equipment. I too master recordings at 24-bit depth. I too hear things other people do not hear (the 60Hz hum in a cathode ray tube while barely discernible to most is glaringly present to the point of annoying me whenever I walk into a room where someone's left a display on with the sound off).

And yet I will bet that you will not be able to correctly identify the 128kbps AAC track in a double-blind test more than 75% of the time. Say whatever else you want, but if you cannot readily identify it more than 75% of the time, then the consistency of your perception, not the fidelity of AAC, needs to be called into question.

A double-blind test requires that neither you nor the person administering the test know which samples are AAC and which samples are PCM. The samples must be repeated enough times to ensure statistical relevancy, and must also be weighed against the results of your selections in a 2nd trial where you are told what the formats are, and repeated again in a trial where you are given false information (i.e. told it's AAC when it's really PCM and vice-versa).

I will also bet that you will frequently erroneously identify the PCM files as having poorer fidelity when you are told falsely they are AAC files.

I listen to my music through either a very expensive set of studio monitors which are designed to make music sound BAD ( or at the very least reveal flaws and imperfections that consumer friendly speakers do not) or a pair of expensive studio headphones.

There are many expensive systems that are mediocre. Price is not always an indicator of great fidelity.

To wit... $3500 Wadia CD players are just repackaged Pioneer transports with Burr-Brown DACs. Granted, the Burr-Brown DAC can make a significant difference but I find it funny that there's a bunch of people out there buying $3500 CD players who would probably thumb their noses at Pioneers.

I will 100% admit you can't hear the difference on iMac speakers, cheap stereo or even ipod headphones but I assure you despite what the scientific graphs and boffins declare the sound is not as pure.

Your assurances mean nothing if your perception is flawed.

I still don't see any evidence of you understanding the fundamental principles of digital audio encoding. Whenever I hear the "AAC is crap" arguments I never, ever, hear from the opponents a dissection of the encoding schema for AAC that might actually result in substantially higher (read: measurable) degrees of interpolation error, quantization noise, aliased/foldover frequencies, elevation of the noise floor, frequency response roll-off, etc.

What it seems to amount to is this bogus perception that the same sound cannot be constructed from fewer data without understanding how such a thing is mathematically possible. As a simple example, consider the difference between Linear PCM and Adaptive Delta PCM (ADPCM). ADPCM is a lossless coding schema. It achieves the same result as PCM but with fewer bits. How? ADPCM only records the difference in amplitude, rather than the absolute value, from one quantization interval to the next. This results in a substantial decrease in the potential bits of data required to reconstruct the exact same analog waveform.

AAC goes several steps further by factoring human perception into account. Like AC-3, there are parameters in AAC that achieve efficiency at tremendously low bitrates because there's a lot of information typically encoded in a PCM stream that will never affect how you perceive the intended analog fundamental.

Show me, for example, how an AAC algorithm fails to reproduce the same amplitude value at a given quantization interval and WHY and then we might actually enter into a discussion of the logarithmic scale and whether or not the difference in amplitude value is large enough to be discernible by the human ear. It very well may be, but all the anecdotes in the world don't begin to take us into a real academic discussion of digital reproduction systems.

Have you read Pohlmann, by any chance?

Regardless of any of that - the bottom line is if the customer thinks there is too much lost they will buy a CD - and that's bad news for Apple!. That's where I am now and it annoys me becuase I would love to buy a ton of stuff from iTunes - I love it and I like it's distribution model but the long term compromise is too great. DRM doesn't bother me one bit!

I think Apple need to tackle this problem in a different way ;

ie You download Lossless or 256AAC but you can if you want SELECT by preference the a bit rate of the tracks that get put onto your ipod. This will allow them to retain the '1000 songs in your pocket' concept but allows the customer to hold a pristine archive copy on their mac/iTunes.

Actually I would even pay a 10% premium for lossless if it helps Apple cover bandwidth bills!, so that people who are happy with 128K AAC can still d/l at standard rate and that I would download at lossless and pay another £0.79 per album say...

Again, given that I will contest the notion that 128kbps AAC is fundamentally discernible from 16-bit PCM, my personal answer to all this has been to use 128kbps AAC for all my casual listening. For critical listening, I don't even use Apple Lossless or 16-bit PCM. I use 24-bit linear PCM which produces substantial gains even the average listener will usually notice. There again is a tremendous difference between the dynamic range of 16-bit and 24-bit PCM . Frequency response should never be a problem as long as the proper considerations of A-weighted sound are taken into account. That is to say that any digital encoding schema designed properly would low-pass filter any frequencies above the Nyquist limit so as to eliminate alias/foldover. This ensures that the frequency response of the reconstructed analog wave is identical to the original.

Things like "tonality" and "presence" are also largely a function of the dynamic range of an encoding schema. Certain types of music that push the boundaries of 16-bit's dynamic range are much better suited at 24-bit which is why I'm a huge proponent of media that support 24-bit. The problem in the hardware world is that manufacturers like Sony create proprietary encoding like DSD (1-bit, 2.7MHz) for proprietary media (SACD) and then refuse to make their players compatible with other optical media (24-bit DVD Audio). This isn't as big a problem in the computer world and even iTunes supports 24-bit PCM.

So if you're going to complain about AAC over "tonality" and "presence" then throw away all your CD's because they're essentially just as worthless.
 
Just to show that some people hear the difference:

This was a ABX test done with 196AAC and Lossless. More people in this thread got it right but here is one:

Linky. 14/16 0.2% chance of guessing.

Here you have your evidence. ;)
 
Just to show that some people hear the difference:

This was a ABX test done with 196AAC and Lossless. More people in this thread got it right but here is one:

Linky. 14/16 0.2% chance of guessing.

Here you have your evidence. ;)

What evidence? That post tells me nothing about the conditions of the test, whether there were placebos involved and what the results with the placebos were. In this case a placebo would be a file type labeled, e.g., AAC when it's really PCM and vice-versa. Also, order randomization, etc.

If the results differ by more than 5 percent when the correct filetype is labeled, and/or when the opposite file type is labeled, then you can throw out the results.

I'm also curious if the lossless file was first or last, whether the same song was used repeatedly or if each step in the trial used different songs, and if the order of formats stayed the same or ws randomly shuffled around. Do you know why?
 
What evidence? That post tells me nothing about the conditions of the test, whether there were placebos involved and what the results with the placebos were. In this case a placebo would be a file type labeled, e.g., AAC when it's really PCM and vice-versa.

Too lazy to explain everything. Read the entire thread. Foobar was used to do the ABX testing, so you can't see which is which. You just match A B X and Y, where A and B stay the same and X and Y are random. 16 matches with the same song.

Edit: You can't see which is which and you're not supposed to say which is AAC and which Lossless but rather to hear a difference between the songs and consistently match them correctly. If you don't know what ABX tests are look it up on Wikipedia.
 
Too lazy to explain everything. Read the entire thread. Foobar was used to do the ABX testing, so you can't see which is which. You just match A B X and Y, where A and B stay the same and X and Y are random. 16 matches with the same song.

Edit: You can't see which is which and you're not supposed to say which is AAC and which Lossless but rather to hear a difference between the songs and consistently match them correctly. If you don't know what ABX tests are look it up on Wikipedia.

And I'm too busy to read a 20+ page thread. I know what ABX testing supposedly is... but I don't know that I trust its methodology given what you've explained.

The problem with the test should be obvious... it's a leading test like multiple choice rather than fill in the blank.

So you hear reference sample A. Then you hear reference sample B.

Then you identify Sample X as either identical to Sample A or B. Does this prove that you can tell which one is AAC and which one is lossless? Absolutely not. In no case has the individual actually knowingly discerned the sample of greatest fidelity. But you can see how the results can be misinterpreted to suggest that.

The real test should be broken into three parts.

The first round would be unlabeled samples played in random order.

The second round would be labeled samples played in random order, but some of the samples are labeled correctly and some of the samples are labeled incorrectly. In this round the objective is to see what the label does to the individual's perception.

In both of the first two rounds, the same song would be repeated for compressed and uncompressed samples and then the round would have to be repeated several times with a different song each time.

So you have something like this (each letter = different song; lowercase = compressed; uppercase = uncompressed):

1. A a
2. b B
3. c C
4. D d
5. E e
6. f F

In the third round, a series of different songs are played only once and you are not given both compressed and uncompressed samples, none of which are labeled, something like this:

1. a
2. B
3. C
4. d
5. E
6. f

In each trial in each of the three phases you would have to identify not which sample sounds like which other sample, but correctly identify which sample is compressed and which sample isn't.

That is an example of a better randomized, double-blind test (double-blind because a computer can distribute the randomization indiscriminately whereas a researcher might provide unintentional cues if they knew which file was which).

If there is statistically significant error in the mean ability to guess correctly in round one, then the claim against perceptual transparency is suspect.

If the mislabeling creates a statistically relevant shift in the mean ability to identify compression accurately, then the claim against perceptual transparency is suspect.

If the ability in round one or round two is not replicated in round three without a difference margin of statistical significance then the claim against perceptual transparency is suspect.

EDIT: One more requirement in a randomized, double-blind test. The individuals for the trial must be selected by a random process and the sample population should be large enough to produce statistically significant results. Posting the test link on a message board of audio enthusiasts is going to invite anything but a random sample population and the results cannot therefore be extrapolated to be indicative of the general public in any meaningful way.
 
It doesn't matter whether you can identify Lossless or AAC. It matters whether you can tell a difference between the two. Because this is what people claim, that there is no difference.
 
Nope, it just says:

Computer Authorizations: 4 machines are authorized to play music purchased with this account.

(2 of those machines no longer exist)

There is no way to change it, that I can see, but I'm VERY impatient. :(

You have to have 5 authorized before you can deauthorize all. Apple sees it as, "If you have one left, why do you need to deauthorize."
 
It doesn't matter whether you can identify Lossless or AAC. It matters whether you can tell a difference between the two. Because this is what people claim, that there is no difference.

It matters absolutely. The counterclaim is that there is a difference and that the difference is specifically that Lossless is perceptibly superior to AAC.

If that claim is to be substantiated, then one can demonstrate the veracity of the claim by showing that people do not fail to consistently identify which file is which.

If their guesses as to which file is which keep changing from one song to the next, from one order to the next, the files being things they have to listen to, then the claim of any consistently audible difference is tenuous at best.

The determination of this is dependent upon the consistency not just from song to song, but the consistency of the results between phase one, two and three of my stated examples.

The three phases are included to rule out any chance of potential biasing by any one of the three methods AND to determine the degree to which the perception is actually consistent.

By not being able to consistently identify which file is which, this means some of the time they hear the opposite of what you are claiming and therefore A is not consistently perceived as better than B.
 
It matters absolutely. The counterclaim is that there is a difference and that the difference is specifically that Lossless is perceptibly superior to AAC.

If that claim is to be substantiated, then one can demonstrate the veracity of the claim by showing that people do not fail to consistently identify which file is which.

If their guesses as to which file is which keep changing from one song to the next, from one order to the next, the files being things they have to listen to, then the claim of any consistently audible difference is tenuous at best.

The determination of this is dependent upon the consistency not just from song to song, but the consistency of the results between phase one, two and three of my stated examples.

The three phases are included to rule out any chance of potential biasing by any one of the three methods AND to determine the degree to which the perception is actually consistent.

By not being able to consistently identify which file is which, this means some of the time they hear the opposite of what you are claiming and therefore A is not consistently perceived as better than B.

If you can hear the difference you can obviously tell which is lossy and which isn't so again, it doesn't matter.

Again, abx tests are the common tests to do double-blind tests. If you are not familiar with that read up:

http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Codec_listening_test
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/ABX_test
http://www.bostonaudiosociety.org/bas_speaker/abx_testing.htm
 
EDIT: One more requirement in a randomized, double-blind test. The individuals for the trial must be selected by a random process and the sample population should be large enough to produce statistically significant results. Posting the test link on a message board of audio enthusiasts is going to invite anything but a random sample population and the results cannot therefore be extrapolated to be indicative of the general public in any meaningful way.

This is the most ridiculous statement ever. Who the hell cares if other people cannot hear the difference. I don't listen to my music for other people so if I can hear the difference I won't use lossy.

That's the same as saying, many people don't have a HDTV, so why offer high def television?
 
If you can hear the difference you can obviously tell which is lossy and which isn't so again, it doesn't matter.

No. Cataloguing a difference doesn't actually identify whether or not the subject can tell WHAT difference there is. Also, if they cannot catalogue the difference to the same degree of effectiveness when NOT given A B reference samples but entirely random samples only once then that calls into question the very ability to objectively discern a difference.

If the difference is more noticeable when subjectively discerned from two copies of the same song in two different formats, but not objectively discernable, then the problem with the claim is that the difference is not consistently perceivable under different conditions. It is the requirement of a scientific randomized double-blind test that it must be able to rule out such bias. In this regard, ABX testing fails to meet the scientific standard.

In other words, if the claim is that lossless is perceptibly better than AAC, then the test should be designed to have the subject identify which is which.

Failing to do so consistently means that the claimed perceivable difference between the files is not consistently perceptible!

Again, abx tests are the common tests to do double-blind tests. If you are not familiar with that read up

Common? Perhaps. Meaningful? No. They fail the scientific standard for double-blind tests because the sample populations are not actively selected at random. The sample populations on online ABX tests are passively determined by whatever demographic group takes interest in such things. Not scientific at all.

If you're not familiar with scientific process or statistical significance, then take a few science and statistics courses. Also, read Ken Pohlmann's Principles of Digital Audio while you're at it.
 
No. Cataloguing a difference doesn't actually identify whether or not the subject can tell WHAT difference there is. Also, if they cannot catalogue the difference to the same degree of effectiveness when NOT given A B reference samples but entirely random samples only once then that calls into question the very ability to objectively discern a difference.

If the difference is more noticeable when subjectively discerned from two copies of the same song in two different formats, but not objectively discernible, then the problem with the claim is that the difference is not consistently perceivable or bordering so closely to the threshold of perception as to render the degree of difference statistically insignificant. It is the requirement of a scientific randomized double-blind test that it must be able to rule out such bias if we are to conclude that the codec is consistently perceived as having greater fidelity. In this regard, ABX testing fails to meet the scientific standard.

So, I'll summarize... if the claim is that lossless is perceptibly better than AAC, then the subject should be able to consistently identify which is which.

Failing to do so consistently means that the claimed perceivable difference between the files is not consistently perceptible!

Just as importantly, you have failed to provide any dissection of encoding fundamentals that would reveal that, perception aside, there actually IS a difference between the reconstruction of an analog waveform from 128kbps AAC versus Lossless (AAC VBR) or even 16-bit LPCM.

Seriously, it is getting tiring explaining this. If you don't want to understand the concept of the test or the original claim that you reiterated in this post, that there is no difference between the two, then I give up. I have shown you that some people can hear a difference between the two.

So to conclude this, keep on thinking whatever you want and I'll have my peace. Thanks.
 
Register on MacRumors! This sidebar will go away, and you'll see fewer ads.